Open Access
Issue
OCL
Volume 23, Number 1, January-February 2016
Article Number D106
Number of page(s) 9
Section Dossier: Lipids and Brain / Lipides et cerveau
DOI https://doi.org/10.1051/ocl/2015060
Published online 27 November 2015

© J.Y. Bernard et al., Published by EDP Sciences, 2015

Licence Creative CommonsThis is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

1 Introduction

The two series of polyunsaturated fatty acids (PUFA), the omega-3 (n-3) and omega-6 (n-6) PUFA, are essential fatty acids which must be provided by the diet and play important biological roles in metabolism, membrane structure and cell signalling, especially in nervous system (German, 2011). Animal studies have greatly participated to improve our understanding of PUFA-related mechanisms (Innis, 2000). It is now clear that dietary deficiency in n-3 PUFA may affect neural system functions in rodents (Bourre et al., 1989), but also behaviour and cognition in non-human primates (Hsieh and Brenna, 2009, Pifferi, 2014). Nevertheless, metabolic pathways and nutrient requirements differ across species, limiting to fully extend inputs from animal studies to humans (Innis, 2000).

Studies in humans have shown that PUFA, and particularly the long-chain PUFA (LC-PUFA) arachidonic acid (AA, n-6 LCPUFA) and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, n-3 LCPUFA), rapidly accumulates into foetus brain during the last trimester of pregnancy (Clandinin et al., 1980a), a phenomenon continuing over the first two years after birth (Clandinin et al., 1980b). A scientific consensus has since emerged on the requirements of LC-PUFA during pregnancy, lactation and infancy, now clearly identified as a critical period for brain development (Koletzko et al., 2008), especially in preterm infants (Lapillonne et al., 2013). Despite evidences on the link between LC-PUFA and human brain development, most randomized controlled trials have failed to prove the benefit of LC-PUFA on child’s psychomotor development. Indeed, recent meta-analyses and systematic reviews have concluded to no clear benefit on child’s psychomotor development of supplementing both term and preterm infants with formulas enriched in LC-PUFA (Schulzke et al., 2011; Simmer et al., 2011). Trials supplementing pregnant or lactating women with fish-oil rich in LC-PUFA have not resulted in more evidence regarding similar outcomes in children (Delgado-Noguera et al., 2010; Gould et al., 2013).

Nevertheless, many observational studies have found a positive association between breastfeeding and psychomotor development (Anderson et al., 1999; Brion et al., 2011), few studies highlighting a dose-effect relationship when considering duration and intensity of breastfeeding (Belfort et al., 2013; Bernard et al., 2013a). Whether this link is causal remains controversial because of sociodemographic differences between breastfeeding and non-breastfeeding mothers (Der et al., 2006). Yet, such findings may also be attributed to LC-PUFA contents in milk, which are much higher in humans than among other mammals species (Zou et al., 2013), despite variability across human populations (Brenna et al., 2007). To date, few large observational studies have investigated associations of breast milk PUFA contents with child’s psychomotor development: Guxens et al. (2011)showed a negative association between the total n-6/n-3 ratio in colostrum and mental development, but only in infants who were breastfed the longest time.

Observational studies having focused on prenatal period, have assessed foetal exposure to PUFA either with PUFA levels measured in cord blood, either by using maternal fish and seafood consumption during pregnancy as a proxy. One study using cord blood data found a positive association between DHA levels and motor development (Bakker et al., 2009), while two other studies found no relationship with cognitive development (Bakker et al., 2003; Ghys et al., 2002). Regarding maternal fish consumption, findings from existing studies are more consistent by showing positive associations with child’s psychomotor development (Daniels et al., 2004; Hibbeln et al., 2007; Oken et al., 2008). However, this approach by food group has the limitation not to take other dietary sources of n-3 and n-6 PUFA into account. Indeed, there are growing evidence that not only n-3 LCPUFA matters, but also the dietary balance between n-6 and n-3 PUFA, because of the endogenous metabolic competition between these two series (Lands, 2015; Simopoulos, 2011a). Furthermore, this balance has dramatically changed worldwide over the last century, and this could have multiple implications for health in general, and for the developing brain in particular (Simopoulos, 2011b). There is thus a need for studying in a more comprehensive way, the roles of n-6 and n-3 PUFA and LCPUFA in early human life.

In this context, we hypothesized that breastfeeding duration is positively associated to child’s psychomotor development, that higher AA and DHA levels in colostrum may explain this postnatal association, and finally that higher prenatal exposure to AA and DHA through maternal dietary intake during pregnancy may also be beneficial for child’s psychomotor development. Thus, we investigated the associations of pre- and postnatal exposures to PUFA, with child’s psychomotor development, by using prospective data from a large mother-child cohort. This article presents a synthesis of the three main results of this study, previously published in peer-review journals (Bernard et al., 2013a, 2013b, 2015), more extensively detailed in a Ph.D. thesis (Bernard, 2013), and finally presented in March 2015 during the Lipids and Brain III Conference held in Paris, France (Heude and Bernard, 2015).

2 Methods

2.1 Study design

The EDEN study is a French mother-offspring cohort aiming at investigating the roles of pre- and postnatal determinants of child developmentand health. Recruitment of pregnant women started in 2003 in the university hospitals of Poitiers and Nancy and ended in 2006. All women presenting to their first antenatal visit before 24 weeks of amenorrhea were invited to participate in the cohort. Exclusion criteria were multiple pregnancies, known diabetes prior to pregnancy, illiteracy, and intention to move outside the region in the next 3 years. A total of 2002 women were enrolled. More details on the study protocol are available (Heude et al., 2015). Informed written consents concerning the parents were obtained at enrolment, and that for the child was acquired after birth. The study was approved by the ethics research committee (Comite Consultatif de protection des personnes dans la recherche biomedicale) of the Bicetre Hospital, and by the Data Protection Authority (Commission Nationale de l’Informatique et des Libertés).

2.2 Assessment of exposure to PUFA

2.2.1 Dietary intake during pregnancy

Maternal dietary intake during the last trimester of pregnancy was evaluated within few days after delivery using a food frequency questionnaire (FFQ) combined to a portion-size picture booklet. The FFQ was adapted from the one used in the Fleurbaix-Laventie Ville Santé study, by adding items concerning seafood consumption and other foods rich in vitamin A and B9, and in n-3 PUFA (Lauzon et al., 2004; Deschamps et al., 2009). Using a food composition database (SU.VI.MAX, 2006), we estimated maternal dietary intakes in LA, AA, α-linolenic acid (ALA), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA), docosapentaenoic acid (DPA), and DHA, and we calculated total dietary intakes in n-6 and n-3 PUFA. We derived dietary ratios between n-6 and n-3 series: total n-6/n-3, LA/ALA and AA/DHA. More details on this method have been published elsewhere (Bernard et al., 2013b; Drouillet et al., 2009).

2.2.2 Breastfeeding

Infant’s feeding modes from birth to maternity discharge were obtained from medical records. In the questionnaire mailed at 4 months, parents reported infant’s consumption of breast milk, infant formulas, cow’s milk, water and other fluids, and solids over periods defined as following: first week, second to fourth weeks, second month, third month and fourth month. In questionnaire mailed at 8 months, 12 months and 24 months, mothers answered the question: “Do you still breastfeed your infant?” and use of infant formulas was also recorded. Mothers who had stopped breastfeeding reported the date of full weaning. Duration of ‘any breastfeeding’ (including partial and exclusive breastfeeding, in months) was derived from the date of birth and the date of full weaning with an accuracy to the day. Duration of ‘exclusive breastfeeding’(in months) was estimated from answers about infant’s feeding mode. We defined ‘exclusive breastfeeding’ as not receiving formulas, as few infants (5%) received other liquids or food in addition to breast milk. Infants (n = 166) who received formulas for medical reasons during their stay at maternity unit, and who were exclusively breastfed after discharge were considered as exclusively breastfed. Infant’s ‘ever breastfed’ were defined as having been initiated to breast milk at some time or other. It is important to note that infant formulas sold in France at that time were rarely supplemented in LC-PUFA. More details on breastfeeding practices in the EDEN study have been published (Bonet et al., 2013).

2.2.3 Composition of colostrum in fatty acids

About 5 ml of colostrum were collected in mothers who breastfed their child during the first week after delivery. Analysis of fatty acid composition was performed by direct transmethylation and fast gas chromatography. More details are available elsewhere (Bernard et al., 2015). Fatty acids were expressed as proportion of total milk fat (wt% of total fatty acids).Twelve well represented PUFA were identified: linoleic acid (LA, 18:2 n-6), γ-linolenic acid (18:3 n-6), dihomo-γ-linolenic acid (20:3 n-6), arachidonic acid (AA, 20:4 n-6), adrenic acid (22:4 n-6), n-6 docosapentaenoic or osbond acid (22:5 n-6), α-linolenic acid (ALA, 18:3 n-3), stearidonic acid (18:4 n-3), eicosatetraenoic acid (20:4 n-3), eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA, 20:5 n-3), n-3 docosapentaenoic or clupanodonic acid (22:5 n-3), and docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, 22:6 n-3). We calculated the total levels in n-6 PUFA and in n-3 PUFA and derived ratios between PUFA: total n-6/n-3 ratio, LA/ALA and AA/DHA.

2.3 Assessment of child cognitive development

In parental questionnaire mailed at two years of age, child’s motor development was assessed using 22 motor-related items from the French Psychomotor Developmental Scale for Early Childhood of Brunet-Lézine (Josse, 1997). Items were summed to obtain a score of motor development (Motor-2) comprised between 0 and 22. Still in two-year parental questionnaire, child’s language ability was evaluated using the short form of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (CDI-2) (Fenson et al., 1993), adapted in French by Kern (2003). Parents reported from a list of 100 words, those that the child was able to spontaneously pronounce. CDI-2 score ranged between 0 and 100. At three years, child’s cognition was investigated with the second French edition of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire (ASQ-3) (Squires et al., 1999). ASQ-3is a parent-reported assessment which includes five domains of development (communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving and personal-social). For each of the 30 questions, a child scored 10 points when parents reported that the child’s ability was acquired, 5 points when the ability was occasionally observed, and 0 point otherwise. ASQ-3 score ranged between 0 and 300 points.

2.4 Population selection

A total of 1907 children was born and included in the cohort. We excluded from the subsequent analysis the infants born before 33 weeks of gestation (n = 12), since early preterm infants are more at-risk of later developmental delay than infants born at term or late preterm. Among the mother-child pairs having either dietary fatty acids data, either colostrum fatty acid data, 1367 children had at least one available psychomotor assessment at 2 or 3 years of age (1260 for Motor-2, 1277 for CDI-2 and 1157 for ASQ-3).

2.5 Statistical analyses

Main characteristics of the population, average dietary PUFA intake and colostrum PUFA levels, and psychomotor development scores were described by means ± standard deviations and percentages. Multivariable linear regression was performed to evaluate whether child’s psychomotor scores were associated with three proxies of early PUFA exposures:

  1. Proxy of duration of postnatal exposure: breastfeeding status, among all children, and breastfeeding durations, among breastfed children.

  2. Proxy of prenatal exposure: maternal dietary PUFA intake during pregnancy, among all children, then separately according to breastfeeding status (ever vs. never breastfed).

  3. Proxy of qualitative postnatal exposure: colostrum PUFA levels, among breastfed children.

All models were adjusted for the following covariates: study centre, child’s sex, exact age at child’s assessment, gestation length, maternal age, primiparity, pre-pregnancy maternal body mass index, smoking status and alcohol consumption during pregnancy, parental education, household income, main caregiver of the child at two years, and frequency of mother-child activities. Models explaining ASQ-3 were additionally adjusted for duration of preschool attendance at three years. Models on maternal dietary intake were additionally adjusted for total energy intake during pregnancy. Models on colostrum PUFA levels were additionally adjusted for day of colostrum collection after delivery, and exclusive breastfeeding duration.

As these results have been previously published, we here only report the main findings to be put into perspective. In a last analysis, we compared adjusted psychomotor scores in never breastfed children and in ever breastfed children as two groups separated by the median (9.7% of total fatty acids): the lowest LA levels vs. the highest LA levels.

All statistical analyses were performed with a two-sided alpha risk of 5%, and conducted using SAS 9.3 software (SAS Institute, Cary, NC).

Table 1

Characteristics of the EDEN study population.

Table 2

Associations of dietary LA intake, n-6/n-3 and LA/ALA ratios during pregnancy with child’s psychomotor development at 2 and 3 years in the EDEN study, according to child’s breastfeeding status.

3 Results

Main characteristics of the 1367 mother-child pairs are reported in Table 1. At inclusion, pregnant women were on average 29.4 ± 4.7years old, and 46.6% were primiparous. Almost 7% were obese before pregnancy, and 22.5 smoked during pregnancy. Children were born on average at 39.3 ± 1.5weeks of gestation, 52.5% were boys, and 76.5% were ever breastfed. Daily total n-6 PUFA and DHA intakes of pregnant women were respectively 9.64 ± 4.4 and 0.17 ± 0.11g/d. In colostrum, total n-6 PUFA and DHA represented respectively 12.2 ± 1.8 and 0.64 ± 0.19% of total fatty acids.

Ever breastfed children scored higher for all psychomotor scores than never breastfed children (results not shown, see Bernard et al. (2013a)). Among ever breastfed children, both exclusive and any breastfeeding durations were positively and significantly associated with psychomotor scores (results not shown).

Table 3

Associations of total n-6, LA levels, n-6/n-3 and LA/ALA ratios in colostrum with child’s psychomotor development at 2 and 3 years among breastfed children of the EDEN study.

Associations between maternal dietary PUFA intake during pregnancy and child’s psychomotor scores are presented in Table 2. Among all children, total n-6/n-3 ratio was negatively associated with Motor-2 (−0.06 ± 0.03, P = 0.04) and ASQ-3 (−0.89 ± 0.36, P = 0.01) but not with CDI-2 (P = 0.4). However, when focusing on never breastfed children only, CDI-2 score was negatively associated with total n6/n3 ratio (−2.1 ± 0.7, P = 0.002). Same patterns of associations were observed with LA intake and with LA/ALA ratio. Overall, the strength of associations was higher in never breastfed than in ever breastfed children for the three psychomotor scores.

thumbnail Fig. 1

Values are adjusted mean ± SEM. Panels A, B and C respectively represent Motor-2, CDI-2 and ASQ-3 scores. P-values thresholds are as follow: *, **, ***. The lowest LA levels are comprised between 5.4 and 9.7% of total fatty acids; the highest LA levels are comprised between 9.7 and 15.9% of total fatty acids. Models were adjusted for study centre, child’s sex and age at assessment, duration of gestation, maternal age, primiparity, maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index, smoking status, alcohol consumption, parental education level, household income, child’s caregivers at 2 years, exclusive breastfeeding duration. Abbreviations: LA, linoleic acid; Motor-2, motor development at 2 years; CDI-2, communicative development inventory at 2 years; ASQ-3, ages and stages questionnaire at 3 years; SEM, standard error of the mean.

Associations between colostrums PUFA levels and child’s psychomotor scores are shown in Table 3. Total n-6 levels were negatively and significantly associated with Motor-2 (−0.10 ± 0.05, P = 0.04) and ASQ-3 (−1.06 ± 0.55, P = 0.05) scores, but not with CDI-2 (P = 0.2). These associations were mostly driven by colostrum LA levels, as indicated by Figure 1, comparing psychomotor scores of never breastfed children to scores of two groups of breastfed children: those fed with the lowest colostrum LA levels, and those fed with the highest LA levels (separated according to the median in LA levels). The group ‘lowest LA levels’ scored significantly higher than the group ‘highest LA levels’ on Motor-2 (P< 0.01), CDI-2 (P< 0.05) and ASQ-3 (P< 0.01). They also scored higher than the group ‘never breastfed’ on Motor-2 (P< 0.001) and ASQ-3 (P< 0.001), but not on CDI-2. Last, no difference on Motor-2 and CDI-2 scores was observed between the groups ‘highest LA levels and ‘never breastfed’.

4 Discussion

As hypothesized, we found a positive association between breastfeeding duration and child’s psychomotor scores in the children of the EDEN mother-child cohort. However, we found no association of prenatal nor postnatal exposure to the LC-PUFA AA and DHA, with psychomotor development. Instead, we found negative relationships with either then-6/n-3 ratio, either total n-6 PUFA, principally driven by LA levels, and this was consistent with prenatal and postnatal assessments. Importantly, children exposed to colostrum with the highest LA levels tended to have psychomotor scores closer to the never breastfed children than to the children breastfed with lower LA levels.

The reliability of these results is supported by strengths inherent to the EDEN cohort. Its prospective design starting from mid-pregnancy enabled to accurately measure several pathways of exposure to PUFA in early life, which increases the consistency of our findings. The amount of data collected in the cohort allowed to control for many potential confounders, although residual confounding may remain. The sample size finally allowed this analysis to be powerful enough to highlight modest differences in child’s psychomotor scores, as assessed at an early age. Nonetheless, this study has limitations. First, psychomotor assessments were parent-reported and not assessed by psychologists. Although these tools are good predictors of standardized psychological tests, it may have introduced observer bias. Second, no measure of maternal intelligence was available in the cohort. Yet, this proxy of both genetic and environmental inheritances has been previously described as a potential confounder (Jacobson and Jacobson, 2002). Last, the EDEN study was not representative of the general population, participants having a higher socioeconomic status, which limits its external validity.

The observed association between breastfeeding and child’s psychomotor development was reported by many previous articles (Anderson et al., 1999), while whether the link is causal is still in debate (Der et al., 2006). In our study, we showed a dose-effect relationship, which is one criteria for arguing in favour of causation (Hill, 1965). Using data from a large cluster-randomized controlled trial, Kramer et al. (2008) have provided further evidence on causation by reporting higher cognitive abilities in children born in maternities which had experienced a promotion for breastfeeding. Whereas it remains difficult to definitively conclude on the causal nature of this link (Walfisch et al., 2013), we have undertaken to test whether AA and DHA contained in breast milk could explain or mediate the associationsbetween breastfeeding and child’s psychomotor development. Using data of colostrum fatty acid content (exhibiting relatively high levels of DHA and AA), we found no evidence of such a benefit of LC-PUFA in breast milk. Conversely, we found negative associations between colostrum total n-6 fatty acids, especially in LA, and child’s outcomes, which is in accordance with a previous study (Sabel et al., 2012). Previously, Guxens et al. (2011) had found that the association between the total n-6/n-3 PUFA ratio in breast milk and infant’s mental development was moderated by the cumulative intensity of breastfeeding. In our study, we did not report this interaction. Maternal dietary LA intake during pregnancy was also associated with poorer psychomotor development among never breastfed children. This could be explained by the metabolic competition between n-6 and n-3 series for the same desaturases and elongases. Indeed, studies have reported that elevated n-6 PUFA intake may decrease the metabolic activity of the n-3 family, which might in turn decrease the synthesis and the accretion of DHA into the developing brain (Novak et al., 2008; Simopoulos, 2011b). Recently, studies have investigated the roles of single nucleotide peptides located on genes involved in the metabolism of PUFA, especially the FADS cluster (Lattka et al., 2010). As an example, a study has reported that the association between breastfeeding and child cognition was moderated by genetic variant in FADS2 gene (Caspi et al., 2007). The input from gene-environment studies are promising to better understand how pre- and postnatal exposure to PUFA may affect brain development and psychomotor development.

5 Conclusion

We aimed at investigating the link between several measures of exposure to PUFA in early life and child’s psychomotor development at 2 and 3 years in a French prospective mother-child cohort. In our study, we confirmed positive association between breastfeeding duration and psychomotor development, but we found no evidence supporting the hypothesis of a beneficial effect on psychomotor development of both prenatal and postnatal exposure to the LC-PUFA AA and DHA. However, we highlighted negative associations involving total n-6 levels, driven by LA levels, the precursor. This suggests that in early life, an exposure to a too high level of LA, through maternal diet during pregnancy and through breastfeeding, may have adverse effects on child’s psychomotor development. Although this finding needs replication later in childhood and in other large cohorts, it supports current nutritional recommendation of balancing dietary n-6 and n-3, especially during pregnancy and lactation. This also calls for research evaluating potential effects of LA levels found in infant formulas, and highlights the importance of carefully selecting the lipid ingredients used. Finally, this study supports the policies promoting initiation and continuation of breastfeeding.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to the participating families, the midwife research assistants (Lorraine Douhaud, Sophie Bedel, Brigitte Lortholary, Sophie Gabriel, Muriel Rogeon, and Monique Malinbaum) for data collection, and the data entry operators (Patricia Lavoine, Josiane Sahuquillo and Ginette Debotte). We thank Cyrielle Garcia for milk analysis, and Sophie Kern for providing the French version of the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory.

Acknowledgments

Sources of funding. The EDEN mother-child cohort study was supported by: Fondation pour la Recherche Medicale (FRM), French Ministry of Research IFR and Cohort Program, INSERM Nutrition Research Program, French Ministry of Health Perinatality Program, French Agency for Environment Security (AFFSET), French National Institute for Population Health Surveillance (INVS), Paris-Sud University, French National Institute for Health Education (INPES), Nestle, Mutuelle Generale de l’Education Nationale (MGEN), French Speaking Association for the Study of Diabetes and Metabolism (Alfediam), National Agency for Research (ANR non thematic program), Groupes Lipides Nutrition (GNL), and National Institute for Research in Public Health (IRESP TGIR Cohorte Sante 2008 Program). Biological analyses of colostrum samples were funded by the PremUp foundation (foundation for scientific cooperation in connection with pregnancy and prematurity). Study sponsors were not involved in study design, data collection, or data analyses.

Acknowledgments

Disclosure. Authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

References

  • Anderson JW, Johnstone BM, Remley DT. 1999. Breast-feeding and cognitive development: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 70: 525–535. [Google Scholar]
  • Bakker EC, Ghys AJ, Kester AD, Vles JS, Dubas JS, Blanco CE, Hornstra G. 2003. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids at birth and cognitive function at 7 y of age. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 57: 89–95. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bakker EC, Hornstra G, Blanco CE, Vles JS. 2009. Relationship between long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids at birth and motor function at 7 years of age. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 63: 499–504. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, et al. 2013. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years: Effects of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity. JAMA Pediatrics 167: 836–844. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bernard J. 2013. Ph.D. thesis, Faculty of Medicine, Université Paris Sud-Paris XI, Le Kremlin-Bicêtre, France. [Google Scholar]
  • Bernard JY, De Agostini M, Forhan A, et al.,Group EM-CCS. 2013a. Breastfeeding duration and cognitive development at 2 and 3 years of age in the EDEN mother-child cohort. J. Pediatr. 163: 36–42. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bernard JY, De Agostini M,Forhan A, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Charles MA, Heude B, Group EM-CCS. 2013b. The dietary n6:n3 fatty acid ratio during pregnancy is inversely associated with child neurodevelopment in the EDEN mother-child cohort. J. Nutr. 143: 1481–1488. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bernard JY, Armand M, Garcia C, et al.,Group EM-CCS. 2015. The association between linoleic acid levels in colostrum and child cognition at 2 and 3 y in the EDEN cohort. Pediatr. Res. 77: 829–835. [Google Scholar]
  • Bonet M, Marchand L, Kaminski M, et al.,Group EM-CCS. 2013. Breastfeeding duration, social and occupational characteristics of mothers in the French ‘EDEN mother-child’ cohort. Matern. Child Health J. 17: 714–722. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bourre JM, Francois M, Youyou A, et al. 1989. The effects of dietary alpha-linolenic acid on the composition of nerve membranes, enzymatic activity, amplitude of electrophysiological parameters, resistance to poisons and performance of learning tasks in rats. J. Nutr. 119: 1880–1892. [Google Scholar]
  • Brenna JT, Varamini B, Jensen RG, Diersen-Schade DA, Boettcher JA, Arterburn LM. 2007. Docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acid concentrations in human breast milk worldwide. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 85: 1457–1464. [Google Scholar]
  • Brion MJ, Lawlor DA, Matijasevich A, et al. 2011. What are the causal effects of breastfeeding on IQ, obesity and blood pressure? Evidence from comparing high-income with middle-income cohorts. Int. J. Epidemiol. 40: 670–680. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Caspi A, Williams B, Kim-Cohen J, et al. 2007. Moderation of breastfeeding effects on the IQ by genetic variation in fatty acid metabolism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 104: 18860–18865. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Clandinin MT, Chappell JE, Leong S, Heim T, Swyer PR, Chance GW. 1980a. Intrauterine fatty acid accretion rates in human brain: implications for fatty acid requirements. Early Hum. Dev. 4: 121–129. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Clandinin MT, Chappell JE, Leong S, Heim T, Swyer PR, Chance GW. 1980b. Extrauterine fatty acid accretion in infant brain: implications for fatty acid requirements. Early Hum. Dev. 4: 131–138. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Daniels JL, Longnecker MP, Rowland AS, Golding J, Health ASTUoBIoC. 2004. Fish intake during pregnancy and early cognitive development of offspring. Epidemiology. 15: 394–402. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Delgado-Noguera MF, Calvache JA, Bonfill Cosp X. 2010. Supplementation with long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFA) to breastfeeding mothers for improving child growth and development. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 12: CD007901. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Der G, Batty GD, Deary IJ. 2006. Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. BMJ 333: 945. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Deschamps V, de Lauzon-Guillain B, Lafay L, Borys JM, Charles MA, Romon M. 2009. Reproducibility and relative validity of a food-frequency questionnaire among French adults and adolescents. Eur. J. Clin. Nutr. 63: 282–291. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Drouillet P, Forhan A, De Lauzon-Guillain B, et al.. 2009. Maternal fatty acid intake and fetal growth: evidence for an association in overweight women. The ‘EDEN mother-child’ cohort (study of pre- and early postnatal determinants of the child’s development and health). Br. J. Nutr. 101: 583–591. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fenson L, Dale PS, Reznick JS, et al. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User’s Guide and Technical Manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group, 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • German JB. 2011. Dietary lipids from an evolutionary perspective: sources, structures and functions. Matern. Child Nutr. 7: 2–16. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Ghys A, Bakker E, Hornstra G, van den Hout M. 2002. Red blood cell and plasma phospholipid arachidonic and docosahexaenoic acid levels at birth and cognitive development at 4 years of age. Early Hum. Dev. 69: 83–90. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gould JF, Smithers LG, Makrides M. 2013. The effect of maternal omega-3 (n-3) LCPUFA supplementation during pregnancy on early childhood cognitive and visual development: a systematic review and meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 97: 531–544. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Guxens M, Mendez MA, Molto-Puigmarti C, et al. 2011. Breastfeeding, long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in colostrum, and infant mental development. Pediatrics 128: e880–889. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Heude B, Bernard JY. 2015. Early nutritionnal determinants of cognitive development in children of the EDEN mother-child cohort – Role of polyunsaturated fatty acids. In journées chevreul, Lipids & Brain III, Paris, France, March 16-18, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • Heude B, Forhan A, Slama R, et al., Group EM-CCS. 2015. Cohort Profile: The EDEN mother-child cohort on the prenatal and early postnatal determinants of child health and development. Int. J. Epidemiol. (In Press). [Google Scholar]
  • Hibbeln JR, Davis JM, Steer C, et al. 2007. Maternal seafood consumption in pregnancy and neurodevelopmental outcomes in childhood (ALSPAC study): an observational cohort study. Lancet 369: 578–585. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hill AB. 1965. The Environment and Disease: Association or Causation? Proc. R. Soc. Med. 58: 295–300. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hsieh AT, Brenna JT. 2009. Dietary docosahexaenoic acid but not arachidonic acid influences central nervous system fatty acid status in baboon neonates. Prostagland. Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids. 81: 105–110. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Innis SM. 2000. Essential fatty acids in infant nutrition: lessons and limitations from animal studies in relation to studies on infant fatty acid requirements. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 71: 238S–244S. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jacobson SW, Jacobson JL. 2002. Breastfeeding and IQ: evaluation of the socio-environmental confounders. Acta Paediatr. 91: 258–260. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Josse D. 1997. Revised Brunet-Lezine Test: Infancy Psychomotor Development Scale. (Brunet-Lézine Révisé: Echelle de Développement Psychomoteur de la Première Enfance). Paris, France: Etablissement d’Applications Psychotechniques. [Google Scholar]
  • Kern S. 2003. Le compte-rendu parental au service de l’évaluation de la production lexicale des enfants français entre 16 et 30 mois langage en emergence. Glossa 85: 48–62. [Google Scholar]
  • Koletzko B, Lien E, Agostoni C, et al., World Association of Perinatal Medicine Dietary Guidelines Working G. 2008. The roles of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation and infancy: review of current knowledge and consensus recommendations. J. Perinat. Med. 36: 5–14. [Google Scholar]
  • Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, et al., Promotion of Breastfeeding Intervention Trial Study G. 2008. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 65: 578–584. [Google Scholar]
  • Lands B. 2015. Choosing foods to balance competing n-3 and n-6 HUFA and their actions. OCL DOI:10.1051/ocl/2015017. [Google Scholar]
  • Lapillonne A, Groh-Wargo S, Gonzalez CH, Uauy R. 2013. Lipid needs of preterm infants: updated recommendations. J. Pediatr. 162: S37–47. [Google Scholar]
  • Lattka E, Illig T, Koletzko B, Heinrich J. 2010. Genetic variants of the FADS1 FADS2 gene cluster as related to essential fatty acid metabolism. Curr. Opin. Lipidol. 21: 64–69. [Google Scholar]
  • Lauzon B (de), Romon M, Deschamps V, et al. 2004. The Three-Factor Eating Questionnaire-R18 is able to distinguish among different eating patterns in a general population. J. Nutr. 134: 2372–2380. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Novak EM, Dyer RA, Innis SM. 2008. High dietary omega-6 fatty acids contribute to reduced docosahexaenoic acid in the developing brain and inhibit secondary neurite growth. Brain Res. 1237: 136–145. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Oken E, Radesky JS, Wright RO, et al. 2008. Maternal fish intake during pregnancy, blood mercury levels, and child cognition at age 3 years in a US cohort. Am. J. Epidemiol. 167: 1171–1181. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Pifferi F. 2014. Omega-3 PUFA supplementation differentially affects behavior and cognition in the young and aged non-human primate Grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus). OCL 21: A104. [CrossRef] [EDP Sciences] [Google Scholar]
  • Sabel KG, Strandvik B, Petzold M, Lundqvist-Persson C. 2012. Motor, mental and behavioral developments in infancy are associated with fatty acid pattern in breast milk and plasma of premature infants. Prostagland. Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 86: 183–188. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Schulzke SM, Patole SK, Simmer K. 2011. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in preterm infants. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD000375. [Google Scholar]
  • Simmer K, Patole SK, Rao SC. 2011. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. CD000376. [Google Scholar]
  • Simopoulos AP. 2011a. Importance of the omega-6/omega-3 balance in health and disease: evolutionary aspects of diet. World Rev. Nutr. Diet. 102: 10–21. [Google Scholar]
  • Simopoulos AP. 2011b. Evolutionary aspects of diet: the omega-6/omega-3 ratio and the brain. Mol. Neurobiol. 44: 203–215. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Squires J, Potter L, Bricker D. The ASQ user’s guide for the Ages and Stages Questionnaires: A Parent-Completed Child-Monitoring System. In: Paul H. ed., 2nd edition, Baltimore, Maryland: Brookes Publishing Co Inc, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  • SU.VI.MAX. 2006. Table de composition des aliments (Food Composition Tables). Paris: Inserm/Economica. [Google Scholar]
  • Walfisch A, Sermer C, Cressman A, Koren G. 2013. Breast milk and cognitive development – the role of confounders: a systematic review. BMJ Open 3: e003259. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zou X, Huang J, Jin Q, et al. 2013. Lipid Composition Analysis of Milk Fats from Different Mammalian Species: Potential for Use as Human Milk Fat Substitutes. J. Agric. Food Chem. 61: 7070–7080. [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Cite this article as: Jonathan Y. Bernard, Martine Armand, Anne Forhan, Maria De Agostini, Marie-Aline Charles, Barbara Heude, on behalf of the EDEN mother-child cohort study group. Early life exposure to polyunsaturated fatty acids and psychomotor development in children from the EDEN mother-child cohort. OCL 2016, 23(1) D106.

All Tables

Table 1

Characteristics of the EDEN study population.

Table 2

Associations of dietary LA intake, n-6/n-3 and LA/ALA ratios during pregnancy with child’s psychomotor development at 2 and 3 years in the EDEN study, according to child’s breastfeeding status.

Table 3

Associations of total n-6, LA levels, n-6/n-3 and LA/ALA ratios in colostrum with child’s psychomotor development at 2 and 3 years among breastfed children of the EDEN study.

All Figures

thumbnail Fig. 1

Values are adjusted mean ± SEM. Panels A, B and C respectively represent Motor-2, CDI-2 and ASQ-3 scores. P-values thresholds are as follow: *, **, ***. The lowest LA levels are comprised between 5.4 and 9.7% of total fatty acids; the highest LA levels are comprised between 9.7 and 15.9% of total fatty acids. Models were adjusted for study centre, child’s sex and age at assessment, duration of gestation, maternal age, primiparity, maternal pre-pregnancy body mass index, smoking status, alcohol consumption, parental education level, household income, child’s caregivers at 2 years, exclusive breastfeeding duration. Abbreviations: LA, linoleic acid; Motor-2, motor development at 2 years; CDI-2, communicative development inventory at 2 years; ASQ-3, ages and stages questionnaire at 3 years; SEM, standard error of the mean.

In the text

Current usage metrics show cumulative count of Article Views (full-text article views including HTML views, PDF and ePub downloads, according to the available data) and Abstracts Views on Vision4Press platform.

Data correspond to usage on the plateform after 2015. The current usage metrics is available 48-96 hours after online publication and is updated daily on week days.

Initial download of the metrics may take a while.